AIA Green - Family Health Care News

Brain training games won‘t help children do better at school

A quick game on the way to school won‘t help ace the maths test. Boy on tablet by Twin Design/Shutterstock

There has been a big increase recently in the number of computerised “brain training” programs marketed at young children. These programs make impressive claims – that they can help children learn better, that they improve children’s focus and memory, and that they can help children succeed in school.

There’s no doubt that brain training is big business. But scientific evidence suggests that these claims are premature. These programs can help train children at specific tasks, but there is little evidence that this has an impact on their performance in maths, reading or other every-day activities.

Working memory training

Many of these brain training programmes target improvements in working memory. Working memory is a cognitive skill that allows us to store and mentally manipulate information in our heads. It’s crucial for learning, and for all kinds of every day situations. For example, we use working memory to follow conversations, or when remembering what to buy from the supermarket.

Working memory has a limited capacity. This means that there is a limit on how much information we can hold in our minds at any one time. We know that this capacity varies between people. Children in particular have a very small capacity that increases gradually throughout childhood.

The idea behind brain training is similar to the way we strengthen a muscle through exercise: by constantly challenging working memory by playing difficult games, we may be able to increase its capacity. This in turn should lead to improvements in other skills that rely on working memory, such as mathematics, reading, and academic achievement. That’s the idea – but does it work?

There has been much scientific interest in whether working memory can be improved through training. A good training study will meet two important criteria. First, it will assess whether training leads to general improvements in working memory – in other words, do children get better on all working memory tasks, or only on the ones they’ve been trained on?

Second, it’s important that the trained children are compared to an active control group. The active control group should complete similar tasks to the training group, except that their tasks shouldn’t require any working memory. This allows us to distinguish between improvements that are caused by training, and improvements caused by less interesting factors such as increased motivation, or practice at interacting with a computer.

Studies using these rigorous criteria have shown reliable, short-term improvements in children’s working memory following training. These studies have used a variety of different computerised training programmes, and have tested children from five to 18 years of age.

Limited transfer to the classroom

It seems, then, that working memory can be trained. But very few studies have examined whether these training improvements actually extend to improvements in everyday behaviours that rely on working memory. This is a problem, since the usefulness of working memory training programs will depend upon them being able to lead to improvements in real-life behaviour.

There is some limited evidence that working memory training may improve reading ability in seven to nine-year-olds. But a second study in 2013 that analysed performance on a variety of tasks found that there was no benefit of brain training to everyday skills that rely upon working memory.

In this study, Darren Dunning and colleagues at the University of Cambridge gave seven to nine-year-olds up to 25 sessions of either CogMed working memory training or active control tasks. Then they measured whether training improved performance on additional measures of working memory, and broader skills including mathematics, reading, writing and classroom-based skills (such as following instructions). They found improvements on working memory. Crucially, however, these improvements did not extend to improvements on any of the broader skills.

The authors concluded that the benefits of training may be restricted only to tasks that are very similar to the tasks children have been trained on. This may be because training works by helping children to develop strategies (such as using visual imagery, or verbally rehearsing the items that they need to be remember on a task), that are then difficult to apply to common everyday activities.

Pinch of salt

This study sheds important new light on the topic of brain training, since it suggests there are major limitations on the utility of working memory training. Further research is needed examining the effect of training on academic and everyday behavioural and learning measures.

We know that working memory training can improve working memory. Therefore, part of this research should involve determining how training improves working memory in the first place – for example, is it through strategy use, increased capacity, or an increase in the ability to process information quickly? This will allow us to understand definitively whether it is possible to extend these improvements to other skills.

Until we know the answers to these questions, we should take the impressive claims of brain training companies with a pinch of salt.

The Conversation

Emma Blakey receives funding from the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) and the Wellcome Trust.

Other news

Some brain training programs are backed by evidence. Here‘s how to pick them

Some brain training programs are backed by evidence. Here‘s how to pick themBrain training programs support healthy brain ageing – but you‘ve got to choose the right one. from www.shutterstock.com Brain training has been touted as a way to prevent age-related cognitive decline. Many products are available for purchase. But are any actually effective? We reviewed the merits of peer-reviewed clinical intervention studies t...

Preview: ABC‘s Redesign my Brain with Todd Sampson

Preview: ABC‘s Redesign my Brain with Todd SampsonOver the show‘s three episodes, Todd Sampson tests whether it‘s possible to enhance his mind, using exercises designed by scientists. ABCWe live in an age of great public fascination with minds and brains; books about brain plasticity, for instance, regularly make the bestseller lists. This fascination is not merely the product of our thirst for kn...

A neuroscience-based action plan to deal with stress after El Paso and Dayton shootings

A neuroscience-based action plan to deal with stress after El Paso and Dayton shootingsThe tragedies in El Paso and Dayton bring sadness, grief and stress to many people. Ash T Productions/Shutterstock.comAfter the mass murders in El Paso and Dayton, discussions about improving mental health and strengthening gun control laws may be comforting. Neither affords a plan, however, to address the stress that comes with these events to the...

How to reduce the risk of cognitive decline with age

How to reduce the risk of cognitive decline with ageJne Valokuvaus/ShutterstockResearch into how we can keep our brains healthy as we age has gained momentum in recent years. There is now an increased focus on the changes that we can makes to our health and lifestyle, which may prevent dementia. Here are some things that research has shown reduce a person’s risk of cognitive decline with age. Sex ...

The science, drugs and tech pushing our brains to new limits

The science, drugs and tech pushing our brains to new limitsJonathan Cohen/Flickr, CC BY-NCA recent explosion of neuroscience techniques is driving substantial advances in our understanding of the brain. Combined with developments in engineering, machine learning and computing this flowering has helped us enhance our cognitive abilities and potential. In fact, new research into the extraordinary machine in ...
AIA GREEN - FAMILY HEALTH CARE NEWS
Contact: [email protected]